Cultural dimension of individualism and collectivism and its perceptual and cognitive correlates in cross-cultural research
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.15503/jecs20152.210.225Keywords:
individualism,collectivism, holistic thinking analytic thinking, cultureAbstract
This paper reviews the current findings on the dimension of individualism/collectivism, which might be a useful tool for the comparison of different cultures and for the investigation of the effect of culture as a psychological concept on individual mental processes. The validity and reliability of the concept of the dimension of individualism/collectivism is discussed. The related theory of analytic and holistic thinking is introduced within a framework of extensive comparative research in the field of cross-cultural psychology. Several interesting research designs on cross-cultural differences in cognition and perception are described. The empirical part contains a short report of research conducted on a sample (N=92) of Czech and Czech Vietnamese university students using a scale of horizontal and vertical individualism/collectivism (Bartoš, 2010). The results do not fully support the traditional view of individualistic Europeans and collectivistic Asians.
Downloads
References
Bartoš, F. (2010). Individualismus a kolektivismus v české populaci a jejich souvislosti s narcismem [Individualism and collectivism of Czech population and their Relation to Narcissism]. Sociológia, 2. 134-161.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Breugelmans, S. M., Chasiotis, A., & Sam, D. L. (2012). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bond, M. (1988). Finding universal dimensions of individual variation in multicultural studies of values: The Rokeach and Chinese value surveys. Journal of personality and social psychology, 55(6). 1009-1015.
Bond, M. (2002). Reclaiming the individual from Hofstede's ecological analysis- a 20-year odyssey: comment on Oyserman et al.(2002). Psychological bulletin, 128(1), 73-77.
Briley, D. A., Morris, M. W. & Simonson, I. (2000). Reasons as Carriers of Culture: Dynamic versus Dispositional Models of Cultural Influence on Decision Making. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(2).157-178.
Buchtel, E. & Norenzayan, A. (2008). Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 11(4). 264-273.
Cerha, O. (2013). Kulturní a interkulturní psychologie [Cultural and cross-cultural psychology]. Praha: Univerzita Karlova v Praze.
Cozma, I. (2011). How are individualism and collectivism measured? Romanian Journal of Applied Psychology, 13(1). 11-17.
Čeněk, J., Smolík, J., & Vykoukalová, Z. (in print). Interkulturní psychologie: Vybrané kapitoly [Cross-cultural psychology: Selected chapters]. Brno: Grada.
Deregowski, J. B. (1980). Illusions, patterns and pictures. London: Academic Press.
Doi, T. (1963). Amae: A key concept for understanding Japanese personality structure. In: V R. K. Beardsley, & R. J. Smith (Eds.), Japanese culture: Its development and characteristics (pp. 132-138). London: Routledge.
Duffy, S., Toriyama, R., Itakura, S., & Kitayama, S. (2009). Development of cultural strategies of attention in North American and Japanese children. Journal of experimental child psychology, 102(3). 351-359.
Fiske, A. (2002). Using individualism and collectivism to compare cultures--a critique of the validity and measurement of the constructs: comment on Oyserman et al.(2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128(1). 78-88.
Freidingerová, T. (2014). Vietnamci v česku a ve světě [Vietnamese in Czech Republic and in the world]. Praha: SLON.
Gardner, W., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10(4). 321-326.
Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K. & Heyman, S. (1996). The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construal, and individual values on communicationstyles across cultures. Human communication research, 510-543.
Heine, S., Lehman, D., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What's wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales?: The reference-group effect. Journal of personality and social psychology, 82(6). 903-918.
Herskovits, M. J. (1955). Cultural anthropology. New York: Knopf.
Hewstone, M., & Stroebe, W. (2006). Sociální psychologie: Moderní učebnice sociální psychologie [Introduction to social psychology: a European perspective]. Praha: Portál.
Hofstede, G. (1983). The cultural relativity of organizational practices and theories. Journal of international business studies, 14(2).75-89.
Chiu, L.H. (1972). A cross-cultural comparison of cognitive styles in Chinese and American children. International Journal of Psychology, 7(4). 235-242.
Chudzikowski, K., Fink, G., Mayrhofer, W., Minkov, M., & Hofstede, G. (2011). The evolution of Hofstede's doctrine. Cross Cultural Management: An International Journal, 18(1). 10-20.
Jahoda, G. (1992). Crossroads Between Culture and Mind: Continuities and Change in Theories of Human Nature. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kastanakis, M., & Voyer, B. (2013). The effect of culture on perception and cognition: A conceptual framework. Journal of Business Research, 67(4). 425-433.
Kim, U., Yang , K.-S., & Hwang, K.-K. (2006). Indigenous and cultural psychology. New York: Springer Science+Business Media.
Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (2010). Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual review of psychology, 62. 419-449.
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T. & Larsen, J. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3). 201-206.
Kluckhohn, K. (1954). Culture and behavior. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2). 921-976. MA: Addison-Wesley.
Kraus, J., & al. (2007). Nový akademický slovník cizích slov [New academic dictionary of foreign words]. Praha: Academia.
Leung, T., & Kim, M. S. (1997). A revised self-construal scale. Honolulu: University of Hawaii at Manoa.
Levine, T., Bresnahan, M., Park, H., Lapinski, M., Wittenbaum, G., Shearman, S., a další. (2003). Self-Construal Scales Lack Validity. Human Communication Research, 29(2). 210-252.
Lu, L., & Gilmour, R. (2007). Developing a new measure of independent and interdependent views of the self. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1). 249-257.
Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to western theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Markus, H. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological review, 98(2). 224-253.
Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of personality and social psychology, 81(5). 922-934.
Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science, 30(2). 381-399.
Matsumoto, D. (2000). Culture and psychology: People around the world. Belmont (USA): Thomson Learning.
Matsumoto, D. & Yoo, S. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(3). 234-250.
Matsumoto, D., Kudoh, T. & Takeuchi, S. (1996). Changing patterns of individualism and collectivism in the United States and Japan. Culture & Psychology, 2(1). 77-107.
Minkov, M. & Hofstede, G. (2010). Hofstede's fifth dimension: New evidence from the World Values Survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1). 3-14.
Nisbett, R. & Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(19). 11163-11170.
Nisbett, R. & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(10). 467-473.
Nisbett, R., Peng, K., Choi, I. & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological review, 108(2). 291-310.
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E., Kim, B. & Nisbett, R. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(5). 653-684.
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E., Kim, B. & Nisbett, R. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(5). 653-684.
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological bulletin, 134(2). 311-342.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological bulletin, 128(1). 3-72.
Oyserman, D., Kemmelmeier, M. & Coon, H. (2002). Cultural psychology, A new look: reply to Bond (2002), Fiske (2002), Kitayama (2002), and Miller (2002). Psychological bulletin,128(1). 110-117.
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54(9). 741-754.
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, Dialectics, and Reasoning about Contradiction. Science Watch, 54(9). 741-754.
Redfield, R. (1941). The folk culture of Yucatan. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rensink, R., O'Regan, J. & Clark, J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological science, 8(5). 368-373.
Rivers, W. H. (1905). Observations of the senses of the Todas. British Journal of Psychology, 1. 321-396.
Rosaldo, M. Z. (1980). Knowledge and passion: Ilongot notions on self and social life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Segall, M. H., Dasen, P. R., Berry, J. W. & Poortinga, Y. H. (1990). Human behavior in global perspective. Heedham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Shiraev, E. B. & Levy, D. A. (2007). Cross-cultural psychology: critical thinking and contemporary applications (5.edition.). New Jersey: Pearson Education.
Shiraev, E. B. & Levy, D. A. (2013). Cross-Cultural psychology: Critical Thinking and Contemporary Applications. New Jersey: Pearson Education.
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S. & Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: A valid and important dimension of cultural differences between nations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(1). 17-31.
Schwartz, S. (1994). Are There Universal Aspects in the Structure and. Journal of social issues, 50(4). 19-45.
Schwartz, S. (1999). A theory of cultural values and some implications for work. Applied psychology, 48(1). 23-47.
Schwartz, S. H. (1990). Individualism-Collectivism: Critigue and proposed refinements. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21(2). 139-157.
Schwartz, S. & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal content and structure of values: Extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of personality and social psychology, 58(5). 878-891.
Schwartz, S. & Boehnke, K. (2004). Evaluating the structure of human values with confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of research in personality, 38(3). 230-255.
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and social psychological bulletin, 20(5). 580-591.
Singelis, T., Triandis, H., Bhawuk, D. & Gelfand, M. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-cultural research, 29(3). 240-275.
Smith, P., Dugan, S. & Trompenaars, F. (1996). National culture and the values of organizational employees a dimensional analysis across 43 nations. Journal of cross-cultural psychology, 27(2) 231-264.
Steinthal, H. (1858). Der Ursprung der Sprache [The origins of language]. Berlín: Dummler.
Takano, Y. & Osaka, E. (1999). An unsupported common view: Comparing Japan and the US on individualism/collectivism. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2(3). 311-341.
Triandis, H. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51(4). 407-415.
Triandis, H. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of personality, 69(6). 907-924.
Triandis, H. & Gelfand, M. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74(1), 118.
Triandis, H., Leung, K., Villareal, M. & Clack, F. (1985). Allocentric versus idiocentric tendencies: Convergent and discriminant validation. Journal of Research in personality, 19(4). 395-415.
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture. New York: Harper.
Tylor, E. B. (1889). On a method of investigating the development of institutions. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 18. 245-272.
Uskul, A., Kitayama, S. & Nisbett, R. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(25). 8552-8556.
Van de Vliert, E. (2006). Autocratic Leadership Around the Globe Do Climate and Wealth Drive Leadership Culture? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(1). 42-59.
Voronov, M. & Singer, J. (2002). The myth of individualism-collectivism: A critical review. The Journal of Social Psychology, 142(4).461-480.
Wierzbicka, A. (1986). Human emotions: Universal or culture-specific? American Anthropologist, 88(3). 584-594.
Wundt, W. (1900-1914). Völkerpsychologie [The psychology of nations] Leipzig: Engelmann.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
CC-BY
Authors retain copyright and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal. All authors agree for publishing their email adresses, affiliations and short bio statements with their articles during the submission process.